|
|
A similar story (but with some significant differences) may be told
about the end of the very first line of the play (excerpt 3). First, the line
is followed by a 0.056 msec pause; this is less than negligible as an
acoustic cue for line-ending (but, as we shall see, it has a different
function). Secondly, the line ending is indicated by an unusually long,
classical "terminal contour". Third, the last syllable "-tent" is, as
expected, considerably lengthened. It is, indeed, the longest syllable in
this line (0.490 msec), even though, in English, sound sequences in
polysyllables are usually shorter than comparable sequences in
monosyllables (compare, for instance, tailvs. tailor;I have elsewhere
discussed this issue at some length; Tsur, 1998: 156-157). The only
(monosyllabic) word that approximates its duration is now(0.486
msec). The length of this line-initial word is explained by rhetoric, not
rhythmic, reasons. To appreciate the duration of this syllable (of a tri-
syllabic), one might observe that the sequence wint-(in "winter") is
slightly over half as long (0.291). Fourth, the word-final oral stop [t] is
excessively overarticulated (overarticulating, by the same token, the
word boundary and the line boundary as well).
|
|
|
|
|
nowisthewinterofour discontent
Figure 4Wave plot and pitch contour of "Now is the winter of our discontent"
My perception-oriented theory of metre assumes that certain rhythmic
problems can be solved by the overarticulation of certain syllables. All
the gurus who instructed me in empirical research told me they were not
aware of any possibility for the machine to indicate overarticulation. It
seems to me now that the machine canshow overarticulation when,
e.g., certain identifiable features of carefularticulation are slightly or
greatly exaggerated, such as duration; but there are some additional,
quite interesting, features. Language in everyday conversation is
usually underarticulated. Especially in English, certain articulatory
features of word boundaries are almost always suppressed, and words
run into one another. Consider the pairs of back-to-back [s]s in figure
5. The word-final [s] in thisis run into the word-initial [s] in sun.This
is the normal way of speaking.7By contrast, between the word-final
[s] of gloriousand the word-initial [s] of summera minute 0.059 msec
pause is inserted. The listener doesn't perceive it as a pause, but as an
articulatory gesture intended to separate the back-to-back [s]s, a kind of
|
|
|