THE
ETIOLOGY & TREATMENT OF CHILDHOOD
Jordan W. Smoller
University of Pennsylvania
Childhood is a syndrome which has only recently
begun to receive
serious attention from clinicians. The syndrome itself, however,
is not
at all recent. As early as the 8th century, the Persian historian
Kidnom
made references to "short, noisy creatures," who may well have been
what
we now call "children." The treatment of children, however, was
unknown
until this century, when so-called "child psychologists" and "child
psychiatrists" became common. Despite this history of clinical
neglect,
it has been estimated that well over half of all Americans alive today
have experienced childhood directly (Suess, 1983). In fact, the
actual
numbers are probably much higher, since these data are based on
self-reports which may be subject to social desirability biases and
retrospective distortion.
The growing acceptance of childhood as a distinct
phenomenon is
reflected in the proposed inclusion of the syndrome in the upcoming
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition,
or
DSM-IV, of the American Psychiatric Association (1990). Clinicians
are
still in disagreement about the significant clinical features of
childhood, but the proposed DSM-IV will almost certainly include the
following core features:
1. Congenital onset
2. Dwarfism
3. Emotional lability and
immaturity
4. Knowledge deficits
5. Legume anorexia
Clinical Features of Childhood
Although the focus of this paper is on the efficacy
of conventional
treatment of childhood, the five clinical markers mentioned above merit
further discussion for those unfamiliar with this patient population.
CONGENITAL ONSET
In one of the few existing literature reviews on
childhood, Temple-
Black (1982) has noted that childhood is almost always present at birth,
although it may go undetected for years or even remain subclinical
indefinitely. This observation has led some investigators to
speculate on
a biological contribution to childhood. As one psychologist has
put it,
"we may soon be in a position to distinguish organic childhood from
functional childhood" (Rogers, 1979).
DWARFISM
This is certainly the most familiar marker of childhood.
It is widely
known that children are physically short relative to the population
at
large. Indeed, common clinical wisdom suggests that the treatment
of the
so-called "small child" (or "tot") is particularly difficult.
These
children are known to exhibit infantile behavior and display a startling
lack of insight (Tom and Jerry, 1967).
EMOTIONAL LABILITY AND IMMATURITY
This aspect of childhood is often the only basis
for a clinician's
diagnosis. As a result, many otherwise normal adults are misdiagnosed
as
children and must suffer the unnecessary social stigma of being labelled
a
"child" by professionals and friends alike.
KNOWLEDGE DEFICITS
While many children have IQ's with or even above
the norm, almost all
will manifest knowledge deficits. Anyone who has known a real
child has
experienced the frustration of trying to discuss any topic that requires
some general knowledge. Children seem to have little knowledge
about the
world they live in. Politics, art, and science -- children are
largely
ignorant of these. Perhaps it is because of this ignorance, but
the sad
fact is that most children have few friends who are not, themselves,
children.
LEGUME ANOREXIA
This last identifying feature is perhaps the most
unexpected. Folk
wisdom is supported by empirical observation -- children will rarely
eat
their vegetables (see Popeye, 1957, for review).
Causes of Childhood
Now that we know what it is, what can we say about
the causes of
childhood? Recent years have seen a flurry of theory and speculation
from
a number of perspectives. Some of the most prominent are reviewed
below.
Sociological Model
Emile Durkind was perhaps the first to speculate
about sociological
causes of childhood. He points out two key observations about
children:
1) the vast majority of children are unemployed, and
2) children represent one of the least educated segments of
our society.
In fact, it has been estimated that less than 20% of children have
had
more than fourth grade education.
Clearly, children are an "out-group." Because
of their intellectual
handicap, children are even denied the right to vote. From the
sociologist's perspective, treatment should be aimed at helping assimilate
children into mainstream society. Unfortunately, some victims
are so
incapacitated by their childhood that they are simply not competent
to
work. One promising rehabilitation program (Spanky and Alfalfa,
1978) has
trained victims of severe childhood to sell lemonade.
Biological Model
The observation that childhood is usually present
from birth has led
some to speculate on a biological contribution. An early investigation
by
Flintstone and Jetson (1939) indicated that childhood runs in families.
Their survey of over 8,000 American families revealed that over half
contained more than one child. Further investigation revealed
that even
most non-child family members had experienced childhood at some point.
Cross-cultural studies (e.g., Mowgli & Din, 1950) indicate that
family
childhood is even more prevalent in the Far East. For example,
in Indian
and Chinese families, as many as three out of four family members may
have
childhood.
Impressive evidence of a genetic component of childhood
comes from a
large-scale twin study by Brady and Partridge (1972). These authors
studied over 106 pairs of twins, looking at concordance rates for
childhood. Among identical or monozygotic twins, concordance
was
unusually high (0.92), i.e., when one twin was diagnosed with childhood,
the other twin was almost always a child as well.
Psychological Models
A considerable number of psychologically-based theories
of the
development of childhood exist. They are too numerous to review
here.
Among the more familiar models are Seligman's "learned childishness"
model. According to this model, individuals who are treated like
children
eventually give up and become children. As a counterpoint to
such
theories, some experts have claimed that childhood does not really
exist.
Szasz (1980) has called "childhood" an expedient label. In seeking
conformity, we handicap those whom we find unruly or too short to deal
with by labelling them "children."
Treatment of Childhood
Efforts to treat childhood are as old as the syndrome
itself. Only in
modern times, however, have humane and systematic treatment protocols
been
applied. In part, this increased attention to the problem may
be due to
the sheer number of individuals suffering from childhood. Government
statistics (DHHS) reveal that there are more children alive today than
at
any time in our history. To paraphrase P.T. Barnum: "There's
a child born
every minute."
The overwhelming number of children has made government
intervention
inevitable. The nineteenth century saw the institution of what
remains
the largest single program for the treatment of childhood -- so-called
"public schools." Under this colossal program, individuals are
placed
into treatment groups based on the severity of their condition.
For
example, those most severely afflicted may be placed in a "kindergarten"
program. Patients at this level are typically short, unruly, emotionally
immature,and intellectually deficient. Given this type of individual,
therapy is essentially one of patient management and of helping the
child
master basic skills (e.g. finger-painting).
Unfortunately, the "school" system has been largely
ineffective. Not
only is the program a massive tax burden, but it has failed even to
slow
down the rising incidence of childhood.
Faced with this failure and the growing epidemic
of childhood, mental
health professionals are devoting increasing attention to the treatment
of
childhood. Given a theoretical framework by Freud's landmark
treatises on
childhood, child psychiatrists and psychologists claimed great successes
in their clinical interventions.
By the 1950's, however, the clinicians' optimism
had waned. Even
after years of costly analysis, many victims remained children.
The
following case (taken from Gumbie & Poke, 1957) is typical.
Billy J., age 8, was brought to treatment by his parents.
Billy's affliction was painfully obvious.
He stood only 4'3" high and
weighed a scant 70 lbs., despite the fact
that he ate voraciously.
Billy presented a variety of troubling symptoms.
His voice was
noticeably high for a man. He displayed
legume anorexia, and,
according to his parents, often refused to
bathe. His intellectual
functioning was also below normal -- he had
little general knowledge
and could barely write a structured sentence.
Social skills were also
deficient. He often spoke inappropriately
and exhibited "whining
behaviour." His sexual experience was
non-existent. Indeed, Billy
considered women "icky." His parents
reported that his condition had
been present from birth, improving gradually
after he was placed in a
school at age 5. The diagnosis was "primary
childhood." After years
of painstaking treatment, Billy improved gradually.
At age 11, his
height and weight have increased, his social
skills are broader, and
he is now functional enough to hold down a
"paper route."
After years of this kind of frustration, startling
new evidence has
come to light which suggests that the prognosis in cases of childhood
may
not be all gloom. A critical review by Fudd (1972) noted that
studies of
the childhood syndrome tend to lack careful follow-up. Acting
on this
observation, Moe, Larrie, and Kirly (1974) began a large-scale
longitudinal study. These investigators studied two groups.
The first
group consisted of 34 children currently engaged in a long-term
conventional treatment program. The second was a group of 42
children
receiving no treatment. All subjects had been diagnosed as children
at
least 4 years previously, with a mean duration of childhood of 6.4
years.
At the end of one year, the results confirmed the
clinical wisdom that
childhood is a refractory disorder -- virtually all symptoms persisted
and
the treatment group was only slightly better off than the controls.
The results, however, of a careful 10-year follow-up
were startling.
The investigators (Moe, Larrie, Kirly , & Shemp, 1984) assessed
the
original cohort on a variety of measures. General knowledge and
emotional
maturity were assessed with standard measures. Height was assessed
by the
"metric system" (see Ruler, 1923), and legume appetite by the Vegetable
Appetite Test (VAT) designed by Popeye (1968). Moe et al. found
that
subjects improved uniformly on all measures. Indeed, in most
cases, the
subjects appeared to be symptom-free. Moe et al. report a spontaneous
remission rate of 95%, a finding which is certain to revolutionize
the
clinical approach to childhood.
These recent results suggests that the prognosis
for victims of
childhood may not be so bad as we have feared. We must not, however,
become too complacent. Despite its apparently high spontaneous
remission
rate, childhood remains one of the most serious and rapidly growing
disorders facing mental health professional today. And, beyond
the
psychological pain it brings, childhood has recently been linked to
a
number of physical disorders. Twenty years ago, Howdi, Doodi,
and
Beauzeau (1965) demonstrated a six-fold increased risk of chicken pox,
measles, and mumps among children as compared with normal controls.
Later,
Barby and Kenn (1971) linked childhood to an elevated risk of accidents
--
compared with normal adults, victims of childhood were much more likely
to
scrape their knees, lose their teeth, and fall off their bikes.
Clearly, much more research is needed before we
can give any real hope
to the millions of victims wracked by this insidious disorder.
REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association
(1990). The diagnostic and
statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th edition: A preliminary
report.
Washington, D.C.; APA.
Barby, B., & Kenn, K.
(1971). The plasticity of behaviour. In B.
Barby & K. Kenn (Eds.), Psychotherapies R Us. Detroit: Ronco
press.
Brady, C., & Partridge,
S. (1972). My dads bigger than your dad.
Acta Eur. Age, 9, 123-126.
Flintstone, F., & Jetson,
G. (1939). Cognitive mediation of
labour disputes. Industrial Psychology Today, 2, 23-35.
Fudd, E.J. (1972).
Locus of control and shoe-size. Journal of
Footwear Psychology, 78, 345-356.
Gumbie, G., & Pokey,
P. (1957). A cognitive theory of
iron-smelting. Journal of Abnormal Metallurgy, 45, 235-239.
Howdi, C., Doodi, C., &
Beauzeau, C. (1965). Western
civilization: A review of the literature. Reader's digest, 60,
23-25.
Moe, R., Larrie, T., &
Kirly, Q. (1974). State childhood vs.
trait childhood. TV guide, May 12-19, 1-3.
Moe, R., Larrie, T., Kirly,
Q., & Shemp, C. (1984). Spontaneous
remission of childhood In W.C. Fields (Ed.), New hope for children
and
animals. Hollywood: Acme Press.
Popeye, T.S.M. (1957).
The use of spinach in extreme
circumstances. Journal of Vegetable Science, 58, 530-538.
Popeye, T.S.M. (1968).
Spinach: A phenomenological perspective.
Existential botany, 35, 908-813.
Rogers, F. (1979).
Becoming my neighbour. New York:Soft press.
Ruler, Y. (1923).
Assessing measurements protocols by the
multi-method multiple regression index for the psychometric analysis
of
factorial interaction. Annals of Boredom, 67, 1190-1260.
Spanky, D., & Alfalfa,
Q. (1978). Coping with puberty. Sears
catalogue, 45-46.
Suess, D.R. (1983).
A psychometric analysis of green eggs with
and without ham. Journal of clinical cuisine, 245, 567-578.
Temple-Black, S. (1982).
Childhood: an ever-so sad disorder.
Journal of precocity, 3, 129-134.
Tom, C., & Jerry, M.
(1967). Human behaviour as a model for
understanding the rat. In M. de Sade (Ed.). The rewards
of Punishment.
Paris:Bench press.
FURTHER READINGS
Christ, J.H. (1980).
Grandiosity in children. Journal of applied
theology, 1, 1-1000.
Joe, G.I. (1965).
Aggressive fantasy as wish fulfilment.
Archives of General MacArthur, 5, 23-45.
Leary, T. (1969).
Pharmacotherapy for childhood. Annals of
astrological Science, 67, 456-459.
Kissoff, K.G.B. (1975).
Extinction of learnt behaviour. Paper
presented to the Siberian Psychological Association, 38th annual Annual
meeting, Kamchatka.
Smythe, C., & Barnes,
T. (1979). Behaviour therapy prevents tooth
decay. Journal of behavioral Orthodontics, 5, 79-89.
Potash, S., & Hoser,
B. (1980). A failure to replicate the
results of Smythe and Barnes. Journal of dental psychiatry, 34,
678-680.
Smythe, C., & Barnes,
T. (1980). Your study was poorly done: A
reply to Potash and Hoser. Annual review of Aquatic psychiatry,
10,
123-156.
Potash, S., & Hoser,
B. (1981). Your mother wears army boots: A
further reply to Smythe and Barnes. Archives of invective research,
56,
5-9.
Smythe, C., & Barnes,
T. (1982). Embarrassing moments in the sex
lives of Potash and Hoser: A further reply. National Enquirer,
May 16.