Asa Kasher
Cognitive Pragmatics
Cognitive Pragmatics
Generative linguistics has been portrayed as a branch of cognitive studies of the human mind and brain. Cognitive pragmatics is the study of the linguistic aspects of language use within the methodological and theoretical framework of cognitive science.
The term cognitive science has been broadly used since the early 1970s, to denote studies of the human mind and brain, done mostly by linguists, philosophers, psychologists and neuroscientists, within a certain framework. (For a general history of the field, see Gardner 1985; for the history of its linguistic part, see Chomsky 1982. See also Chomsky 1991a, 1991b.) The major objective of cognitive science is an understanding of the human systems of knowledge, their acquisition, their psychological operation and their neural embodiments. It is assumed that abstract systems of knowledge, such as those of language, perception and reasoning, are characterizable independently of their concrete implementations. The latter are rather characterizable in terms of the former: a calculator is explained as an embodiment of arithmetics, which in turn is not adequately explained in terms of chips and buttons. However, when the psychological aspects of acquisition or operation of any cognitive system are studied, the computer qua information processing device serves as a major model. Psychological and neurological studies of the acquisition, operation and implementation of some abstract, cognitive system have naturally given rise to interdisciplinary studies of the human mind and brain.
Cognitive pragmatics is that part of the cognitive studies of language that is related to some system of pragmatic knowledge of language use (See: CHOMSKY AND PRAGMATICS). The notion cognitive pragmatics has been used since the mid-1980s (Kasher (ed). 1988, preface), but studies that fit this label have been made since the mid-1970s.
Theoretical work in pragmatics that can be incorporated into cognitive pragmatics has been done by philosophers since the 1960s (See Austin 1962, Searle 1969, Grice 1989, Recanati 1987. For a survey, see Levinson 1983). Some philosophical work has been done within cognitive pragmatics, setting a general theoretical framework for cognitive studies of language use (e.g., Kasher 1991a, 1991b, Vanderveken 1990-91. See also PRAGMATICS, MODULAR.) Linguistic theoretical systems have also been developed for the explanation of some major problems in pragmatics (e.g., Sperber and Wilson 1986, Ariel 1990). A unified theory, which explains both the philosophical and the linguistic aspects of language use, is expected to emerge from such separate philosophical and linguistic studies. Linguistic and psychological studies of acquisition of pragmatics have often rested on the assumption that nature language is acquired as a communication device directly related to previously acquired, non-verbal communication devices (e.g., Bates 1976. For a survey, see Carroll 1986. See also Bruner 1986). Cognitive studies that do not rest on this problematic assumption (Kasher 1991a) still remain to be done. There is no general theory of the role played by pragmatic factors in speech comprehension and production. Work has been done on some rather restricted though interesting problems, such as understanding indirect speech acts (e.g., Clark 1979). More work has been done about neuro-psychological aspects of language use. Gardner and others (e.g., Gardner et al. 1983) administered a battery of tests, pragmatic and other ones, to right hemisphere damaged subjects and found interesting deficits, such as in comprehension of indirect speech acts. It is still unclear how such results could be explained within a cognitive framework.
Bibliography
Ariel M 1990 Accessing Noun-Phrase Antecedents. Routledge, London and New York.
Austin J L 1962 How to Do Things with Words. Clarendon Press, Oxford. 2nd edition, Oxford University Press, London, Oxford, New York 1976.
Bates E 1976 Language and Context. Academic Press, New York.
Bruner J 1986 Actual Minds, Possible World. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA and London.
Carroll D W 1986 Psychology of Language. Brooks/Cole, Monterey, California.
Chomsky N 1982 On the Generative Enterprise. A discussion with R Huybregts and H van Riemsdijk. Foris, Dordrecht and Cinnaminson, NJ.
Chomsky N 1991a Linguistics and adjacent fields: A personal view. In: Kasher A (ed). 1991, pp. 3-25.
Chomsky N 1991b Linguistics and cognitive science: Problems and mysteries. In: Kasher A (ed). 1991, pp. 26-53.
Clark H H 1979 Responding to indirect speech acts. Cognitive Psychology 11: 430-477.
Gardner H 1985 The Mind's New Science. Basic Books, New York. 2nd edition, 1987.
Gardner H, Brownell H, Wapner W, Michelow D 1983 Missing the point: The role of the right hemisphere in the processing of complex linguistic materials. In: Perecman E (ed). Cognitive Processing in the Right Hemisphere, Academic Press, New York, pp. 169-191.
Grice P 1989 Studies in the Way of Words. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, and London.
Kasher A (ed). 1988 Cognitive Aspect of Language Use. Special issue of Journal of Pragmatics 12, Nos. 5-6, republished, under the same title, North-Holland, Amsterdam 1989.
Kasher A 1991a Pragmatics and Chomsky's Research Program. In: Kasher A (ed). 1991, pp. 122-149.
Kasher A 1991b On the pragmatic modules: A lecture. Journal of Pragmatics 16: 381-397.
Kasher A (ed). 1991 The Chomskyan Turn. Basil Blackwell, Oxford.
Levinson S C 1983 Pragmatics. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Recanati F 1987 Meaning and Force. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Searle, J R 1969 Speech Acts. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Sperber D and Wilson D 1986 Relevance. Basil Blackwell, Oxford.
Vanderveken D 1990-91 Meaning and Speech Acts, Volumes I-II. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.